Sept. 15, 2005

BACK


Publications in Landscaping, the analysis of Gardens, Perception and Medial Representations by

Gert van Tonder et al. 

Dept. of Architecture and Design,
Faculty of Engineering & Design,
Kyoto Institute of Technology


Landscaping and the Analysis of Gardens:
Perception:
Medial Representations:

BibTeX references.


Recovery of Visual Structure in Illustrated Japanese Gardens

G.J. van Tonder
Pattern Recognition Letters, 2006 (in Press).
Special Issue on Pattern Recognition in Cultural Heritage and in Medical Applications.




Order and Complexity of Naturalistic Landscapes: On Creation, Depiction and Perception of Japanese Dry Rock Gardens

G.J. van Tonder
In Visual Thought: The Depictive Space of the Mind,
Liliana Albertazzi, ed., Benjamins, in Press, 2006+

Proceedings of the Conference "The Depictive Space of Thought," held in Bolzano, Italy, June 2004; organised by MittelEuropa Foundation: http://www.mitteleuropafoundation.it/



Visual Perception in Japanese Rock Garden Design

G.J. van Tonder and M. J. Lyons
Axiomathes, Special Issue on Cognition and Design,
Springer, Vol. 15, 2005 (in Press).




Flexible Computation of Shape Symmetries Within the Maximal Disk Paradigm

G.J. van Tonder and Y. Ejima
IEEE Trans. SMC Part B, 33(3):535-540, June 2003.
(Transactions on Systems, Man and Cybernetics; Part B: Cybernetics)

Abstract

Shape symmetries, like medial axes, contain a wealth of information useful in engineering and many different models for symmetry computation have been proposed. We present the hybrid symmetry transform (HST) as a compactly reformulated version of the maximal disk methods. HST is inspired by shunting inhibition networks and general concepts in wave dynamics. Through adjusting a shunting coefficient, the strength of adherence to the maximal disk paradigm becomes tunable. Each image location is scanned with a set of concentric rings which are then combined via weighted (shunting) summation, as opposed to the winner-takes-all approach in strict maximal disk methods. The new model is simple yet operates flexibly to compute maximal disk symmetries in a variety of conditions, interpreted as different wave propagation modes.


Visual Structure of a Japanese Zen Garden

G.J. van Tonder, M. J. Lyons and Y. Ejima
Nature, no.419, pp. 359-360, Sept. 2002.

Abstract

The dry landscape garden at Ryoanji Temple in Kyoto, Japan, a UNESCO world heritage site, intrigues hundreds of thousands of visitors every year with its abstract, sparse and seemingly random composition of rocks and moss on an otherwise empty rectangle of raked gravel. Here we apply a model of shape analysis in early visual processing to show that the 'empty' space of the garden is implicitly structured and critically aligned with the temple's architecture. We propose that this invisible design creates the visual appeal of the garden and was probably intended as an inherent feature of the composition.

Web links:



The Patchwork Engine: Image Segmentation from Image Symmetries

G.J. van Tonder and Y. Ejima
Neural Networks, Vol. 13, No. 3, pp. 291-303, April 2000.


Fig. 1. Schematic of the patchwork engine:
A visual scene (left upper half) is ideally transformed
into a pattern of patches (right upper half)
which correspond to shape sub-parts.
The schematic shows what ideal patches consist of.

Abstract

We propose blind segmentation of images into shape-related 'patches' based on pre-calculated local symmetries (Van Tonder, G.J. & Ejima, Y. (1999). (Forthcoming a) Flexible computation of shape symmetries. Submitted for publication) in shape boundary contours. First, lateral weights between all points in the boundary contour map are assigned analogous to Euclidean distance maps in watershed segmentation (Beucher, S. & Lantejoul, C. (1979). Use of watersheds in contour detection. Proceedings of the International Workshop on Image Processing, CCETT, Rennes, France.). Lateral weights are then used to: (1) extract local maxima in symmetries; (2) link maxima within locally enclosed boundary contours; and (3) reconstruct shape contours using symmetry maxima as 'seeds'. The new model overcomes weaknesses of watershed segmentation. The new model closes gaps in relatively more solid image contours, but it is fundamentally different from methods based on contour interpolation (Grossberg, S., Mingolla, E. & Todorovc, D. (1989). A neural network architecture for preattentive vision, IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering 36, 65-84; Heitger, F. & von der Heydt, R. (1993). A computational model of neural contour processing: figure-ground segregation and illusory contours. Proceedings of the Fourth International Conference on Computer Vision, IEEE Computer Society Press, Washington D.C. (pp. 32-40)). Images are segmented into shape-relevant color-by-number-like patches which compare well to related methods (Gauch, J. & Pizer, M. (1993). The intensity axis of symmetry and its application to image segmentation, IEEE Transactions on Pattern Analysis and Machine Intelligence, 15 (8), 753-770; Ilg, W. & Ogniewicz, R. (1995). The application of Voronoi skeletons to perceptual grouping in line images, Proceedings of the 11th International Conference on Pattern Recognition, The Hague, The Netherlands, pp. 382-385; Zhu, S.C. & Yuille, A.L. (1996) FORMS: a flexible object recognition and modeling system, International Journal of Computer Vision, 20 (3), 187-212.). Two primitive operations, comparison and merging of patches, are proposed as drives for exposing more global shape contours from patches. We conclude that symmetry goes beyond abstract shape morphology: it can contribute to figure-ground segmentation in early vision and form part of primitive operations needed to create hypotheses of complex shape.


Fig. 2. Hand-drawn reproductions from Blum's (Blum, 1973)
explanation of how (a) superposition versus (b) blocking waves propagate,
and how blocking waves (c) trace out
the symmetry axis of a triangular boundary.
(d) Three different maximal disks, centred
on the symmetry axis (dotted line) of a shape.



Fig. 3. Consider the slug shape above. With ν=3 the maximal disk paradigm,
or blocking wave mode, is strictly followed (A), which fails to give
 more global symmetries of contours when noise elements are introduced (B).
With ν=0.01 the HST operates in between classical and blocking wave modes.
It gives desired symmetries both in the absence (C) and presence of noise (D).
Dark pixels indicate strong symmetry activation. Image contours Image were
superimposed on computed symmetries as black lines and dots.



Fig. 10. More examples of patchwork segmentation. Again, high resolution input
images Image are shown in row A, Image in row B. Image are in row C and
patchwork patterns in row D.



Fig. 11. A texture image (left) and its patchwork (middle).
Patches were shaded according to the average pixel
difference with neighbouring patches (right).
Large differences are plotted darker. ρ=15.


Fig. 12. Input texture images (top), patchwork patterns (middle)
and patch difference (bottom). (D) is obtained via texture element
by element scaling of (A). The visible target in (A) has similar patches,
showing up as a white region (C), whereas the target visible in (D)
is dark (F) due to irregularity of its constituent patches.



Fig. 13. A medical scan image (A) and Canny edge map (B) after (Ilg & Ogniewicz, 1995).
(C) shows the patchwork of (B). (D) and (E) shows 71 patches and a grid
of 81 Cartesian boxes, respectively, with patches and boxes shaded
with the average grey scale value of their overlapping image pixels.





From image segmentation to anti-textons

G.J. van Tonder and Y. Ejima
Perception, volume 29, number 10, pages 1231 - 1247, Oct. 2000.

Abstract

We apply the 'patchwork engine' (PE; van Tonder and Ejima, 2000 Neural Networks forthcoming) to encode spaces between textons in an attempt to find a suitable feature representation of anti-textons [Williams and Julesz, 1991, in Neural Networks for Perception volume 1: Human and Machine Perception Ed. H Wechsler (San Diego, CA: Academic Press); 1992, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 89 6531 - 6534]. With computed anti-textons it is possible to show that tessellation and distribution of anti-textons can differ from that of textons depending on the ratio of texton size to anti-texton size. From this we hypothesise that variability of anti-textons can enhance texture segregation, and test our hypothesis in two psychophysical experiments. Texture segregation asymmetry is the topic of the first test. We found that targets on backgrounds with regular anti-textons segregate more strongly than on backgrounds with highly variable anti-textons. This neatly complements other explanations for texture segregation asymmetry (eg Rubenstein and Sagi, 1990 Journal of the Optical Society of America A 7 1632 - 1643). Second the relative significance of textons and anti-textons in human texture segregation is investigated for a limited set of texture patterns. Subjects consistently judged a combination of texton and anti-texton gradients as more conspicuous than texton-only gradients, and judged texton-only gradients as being more conspicuous than anti-texton-only gradients. In the absence of strong texton gradients the regularity versus irregularity of anti-textons agrees with perceived texture segregation. Using PE outputs as anti-texton features thus enabled the conception of various useful tests on texture segregation. The PE is originally intended as a general image segmentation method based on symmetry axes. With this paper we therefore hope to relate anti-textons with visual processing in a wider sense.


Bottom-up clues in target finding: Why a dalmatian may be mistaken for an elephant

G.J. van Tonder and Y. Ejima
Perception, volume 29, number 2, pages 149 - 157, Feb. 2000.

Abstract

We provide informal psychophysical support for a strategy where bottom - up features guide attention toward a target, and the top - down path interprets hypothetical shapes at the target location -- as opposed to a dominant top - down approach. In our survey, for which we used the familiar picture of a Dalmatian dog against a dappled background, (i) 75% of subjects initially found a bulging body which overlaps that of the dog, but final 'top - down' percepts were unexpected: nearly all subjects assigned an incorrect head and limbs to the body; (ii) after random rotation of texture elements overlapping computed features only 45% of subjects reported a bulging body, with a few adding limbs etc. The picture of the Dalmatian dog must therefore contain many bottom - up features -- a top - down strategy may find 'incorrect' targets at correct target locations. Computational support for these claims is more easily constructed than one may expect. We could compute at least two bottom - up features, both useful in 3-D surface interpolation from 2-D scenes, which yielded significant values at the location of the Dalmatian dog: anisotropic texture compression and affine texture distortion cues. We therefore conclude that the role of top - down processing is overstated in a traditional example such as the Dalmatian dog picture.



BACK

Page created & maintained by Frederic Leymarie, 2005.
Comments, suggestions, etc., mail to: ffl at gold dot ac dot uk